Motor processing modulates word comprehension.

embodied cognition motor processing word comprehension

Journal

British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)
ISSN: 2044-8295
Titre abrégé: Br J Psychol
Pays: England
ID NLM: 0373124

Informations de publication

Date de publication:
Nov 2021
Historique:
revised: 05 04 2021
received: 21 09 2020
pubmed: 30 4 2021
medline: 13 10 2021
entrez: 29 4 2021
Statut: ppublish

Résumé

We evaluated whether movement modulates the semantic processing of words. To this end, we used homograph words with two meanings, one associated with hand movements (e.g., 'abanico', 'fan' in Spanish) or foot movements ('bota', 'boot' in Spanish), and the other not associated with movement ('abanico', 'range' in Spanish; 'bota', 'wineskin' in Spanish). After the homograph, three words were presented, and participants were asked to choose the word related to one of the two homograph meanings. The words could be either related to the motor meaning of the homograph ('fan-heat'), to the non-motor meaning of the homograph ('range-possibility') or unrelated ('fan-phone'). The task was performed without movement (simple condition) or by performing hand (Experiment 1) and foot (Experiment 2) movements. Compared with the simple condition, the performance of movement oriented the preference towards the motor meaning of the homograph. This pattern of results confirms that movement modulates word comprehension.

Identifiants

pubmed: 33914904
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12507
doi:

Types de publication

Journal Article

Langues

eng

Sous-ensembles de citation

IM

Pagination

1028-1052

Informations de copyright

© 2021 British Psychological Society.

Références

Aravena, P., Hurtado, E., Riveros, R., Cardona, J. F., Manes, F., & Ibáñez, A. (2010). Applauding with closed hands: Neural signature of action-sentence compatibility effects. PLoS One, 5, e11751. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011751
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wilson, S. M., Rizzolatti, G., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Current Biology, 16, 1818-1823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060
Bak, T. H., & Chandran, S. (2012). What wires together dies together: Verbs, actions and neurodegeneration in motor neuron disease. Cortex, 48, 936-944. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.07.008
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99532147
Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 1177-1187. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2003.1319
Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716-724. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x
Bergen, B., Lau, T.T., Narayan, S., Stojanovic, D., & Wheeler, K. (2010). Body partrepresentations in verbal semantics. Memory & Cognition, 38, 969-981. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.7.969
Bergen, B., & Wheeler, K. (2010). Grammatical aspect and mental simulation. Brain and Language, 112, 150-158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.07.002
Binder, J. R., & Desai, R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 527-536. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001
Boulenger, V., Roy, A. C., Paulignan, Y., Deprez, V., Jeannerod, M., & Nazir, T. A. (2006). Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 msec of processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1607-1615. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607
Boulenger, V., Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding. NeuroImage, 59, 3502-3513. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.011
Boulenger, V., Silber, B. Y., Roy, A. C., Paulignan, Y., Jeannerod, M., & Nazir, T. A. (2008). Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: A combined EEG and kinematic study. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102, 130-136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.015
Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: A combined TMS and behavioral study. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 355-363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.020
Cuetos, F., Glez-Nosti, M., Barbón, A., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). SUBTLEX-ESP: Spanish word frequencies based on film subtitles. Psicológica, 32, 133-143.
Dalla Volta, R., Fabbri-Destro, M., Gentilucci, M., & Avanzini, P. (2014). Spatiotemporal dynamics during processing of abstract and concrete verbs: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 61, 163-174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.019
de Lafuente, V., & Romo, R. (2004). Language abilities of motor cortex. Neuron, 41, 178-180. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00004-2
de Vega, M., León, I., Hernández, J. A., Valdés, M., Padrón, I., & Ferstl, E. C. (2014). Action sentences activate sensory motor regions in the brain independently of their status of reality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 1363-1376. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00559
Desai, R. H., Binder, J. R., Conant, L. L., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2010). Activation of sensory-motor areas in sentence comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 468-478. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp115
Dutriaux, L., Dahiez, X., & Gyselinck, V. (2019). How to change your memory of an object with a posture and a verb. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(5), 1112-1118. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021818785096
Dutriaux, L., & Gyselinck, V. (2016). Learning is better with the hands free: The role of posture in the memory of manipulable objects. PLoS One, 11, e0159108. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159108
Ellis, A. W., & Young, A. W. (2013). Human cognitive neuropsychology: A textbook with readings. London, UK: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203727041
Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 399-402. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01874.x
Fargier, R., Ménoret, M., Boulenger, V., Nazir, T. A., & Paulignan, Y. (2012). Grasp it loudly! Supporting actions with semantically congruent spoken action words. PLoS One, 7(1), e30663. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030663
Fernández, A., Díez, E., & Alonso, M. A. (2010). Normas de Asociación libre en castellano de la Universidad de Salamanca. Retrieved from www.usal.es/gimc/nalc
Fuster, J. (2015). The prefrontal cortex. London, UK: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407815-4.00002-7
Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G. (2005). The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 455-479. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000310
García, A. M., & Ibáñez, A. (2016). A touch with words: Dynamic synergies between manual actions and language. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 59-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.022
Gentilucci, M. (2003). Object motor representation and language. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 260-265. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1600-8
Gianelli, C., & Dalla Volta, R. (2015). Does listening to action-related sentences modulate the activity of the motor system? Replication of a combined TMS and behavioral study. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1511. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01511
Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., & Cattaneo, L. (2008). Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18, R290-R291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.036
Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301-307. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9
Hauk, O., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Effects of word length and frequency on the human event-related potential. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 1090-1103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.020
Hauk, O., & Tschentscher, N. (2013). The body of evidence: What can neuroscience tell us about embodied semantics? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 50. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00050
Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 291-345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.11.001
Hoenig, K., Sim, E. J., Bochev, V., Herrnberger, B., & Kiefer, M. (2008). Conceptual flexibility in the human brain: Dynamic recruitment of semantic maps from visual, motor, and motion-related areas. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(10), 1799-1814. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20123
Horoufchin, H., Bzdok, D., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2018). Action and object words are differentially anchored in the sensory motor system - A perspective on cognitive embodiment. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-24475-z
Ibáñez, A., Cardona, J. F., Dos Santos, Y. V., Blenkmann, A., Aravena, P., Roca, M., … Bekinschtein, T. (2013). Motor-language coupling: Direct evidence from early Parkinson’s disease and intracranial cortical recordings. Cortex, 49, 968-984. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.02.014
Juárez, F. P. G., Labrecque, D., & Frak, V. (2019). Assessing language-induced motor activity through Event Related Potentials and the Grip Force Sensor, an exploratory study. Brain and Cognition, 135, 103572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.010
Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94, B79-B89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.005
Kiefer, M. (2005). Repetition priming modulates category-related effects on event-related potentials: Further evidence for multiple cortical semantic systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 199-211. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053124938
Kiefer, M., Sim, E. J., Liebich, S., Hauk, O., & Tanaka, J. (2007). Experience-dependent plasticity of conceptual representations in human sensory-motor areas. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 525-542. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.525
Liepelt, R., Dolk, T., & Prinz, W. (2012). Bidirectional semantic interference between action and speech. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 76, 446-455. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0390-z
Lindemann, O., Stenneken, P., Van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2006). Semantic activation in action planning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 633-643. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.633
Linell, P. (2009). Rethinking language, mind, and world dialogically. Charlotte, NC: IAP. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393
Matheson, H. E. (2018). Embodiment and grounding in cognitive neuroscience. In J. Wixted (Ed.), The Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience (4th ed.). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119170174.epcn310
Melloni, M., Sedeño, L., Hesse, E., García-Cordero, I., Mikulan, E., Plastino, A., … Ibáñez, A. (2015). Cortical dynamics and subcortical signatures of motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease. Scientific Reports, 5, 11899. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep11899
Mollo, G., Pulvermüller, F., & Hauk, O. (2016). Movement priming of EEG/MEG brain responses for action-words characterizes the link between language and action. Cortex, 74, 262-276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.10.021
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220-244. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220
Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 976-987. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277
Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (Eds.). (2005). Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499968
Pirog Revill, K. P., Aslin, R. N., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Neural correlates of partial lexical activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 13111-13115. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0807054105
Postle, N., McMahon, K. L., Ashton, R., Meredith, M., & de Zubicaray, G. I. (2008). Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices. NeuroImage, 43, 634-644. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.006
Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 253-279. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X9900182X
Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language y action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 576-582. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706
Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 458-470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004
Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Neural reuse of action perception circuits for language, concepts and communication. Progress in Neurobiology, 160, 1-44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.07.001
Pulvermüller, F., Härle, M., & Hummel, F. (2001). Walking or talking?: Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing. Brain and Language, 78, 143-168. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2000.2390
Pulvermüller, F., Hauk, O., Nikulin, V. V., & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005). Functional links between motor and language systems. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 793-797. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x
Pulvermüller, F., Moseley, R. L., Egorova, N., Shebani, Z., & Boulenger, V. (2014). Motor cognition-motor semantics: Action perception theory of cognition and communication. Neuropsychologia, 55, 71-84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.002
Real Academia Española. (2017). Diccionario de la lengua española [Dictionary of the Spanish Language] (23rd ed.). Madrid, Spain: Author.
RStudio Team. (2016). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio Inc. http://www.rstudio.com/
Sato, M., Mengarelli, M., Riggio, L., Gallese, V., & Buccino, G. (2008). Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing. Brain & Language, 105, 83-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001
Schneider, W. (1995). Micro experimental laboratory. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide (Version 1.1). Pittsburg: Psychology Software Tools.
Shebani, Z., & Pulvermüller, F. (2013). Moving the hands and feet specifically impairs working memory for arm- and leg-related action words. Cortex, 49, 222-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.005
Shebani, Z., & Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Flexibility in language action interaction: The influence of movement type. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 252. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00252
Spadacenta, S., Gallese, V., Fragola, M., & Mirabella, G. (2014). Modulation of arm reaching movements during processing of arm/hand-related action verbs with and without emotional connotation. PLoS One, 9, e104349. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104349
Sprouse, J., & Almeida, D. (2012). Power in acceptability judgment experiments and the reliability of data in syntax (unpublished manuscript). University of California, Irvine & Michigan State University.
Tremblay, P., & Small, S. L. (2011). On the context-dependent nature of the contribution of the ventral premotor cortex to speech perception. NeuroImage, 57, 1561-1571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.067
Tyler, L. K., & Moss, H. E. (2001). Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 244-252. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01651-X
Vukovic, N., Feurra, M., Shpektor, A., Myachykov, A., & Shtyrov, Y. (2017). Primary motor cortex functionally contributes to language comprehension: An online rTMS study. Neuropsychologia, 96, 222-229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.025
Westfall, J. (2016). PANGEA: Power analysis for general anova designs. Unpublished manuscript. Available at http://jakewestfall.org/publications/pangea.pdf
Wickens, C., Kramer, A., Vanasse, L., & Donchin, E. (1983). Performance of concurrent tasks: A psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. Science, 221(4615), 1080-1082. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6879207
Wickens, C. D., & Liu, Y. (1988). Codes and modalities in multiple resources: A success and a qualification. Human Factors, 30, 599-616. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872088803000505
Willems, R. M., Labruna, L., D’Esposito, M., Ivry, R., & Casasanto, D. (2011). A functional role for the motor system in language understanding: Evidence from theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation. Psychological Science, 22, 849-854. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611412387
Zwaan, R. A., Van Der Stoep, N., Guadalupe, T., & Bouwmeester, S. (2012). Language comprehension in the balance: The robustness of the action-compatibility effect (ACE). PLoS One, 7, e31204. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0031204

Auteurs

Giulia Togato (G)

RGRLL Department, California State University Long Beach, California, USA.

Filip Andras (F)

Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), Granada, Spain.
University of Granada, Spain.

Elvira Miralles (E)

Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), Granada, Spain.

Pedro Macizo (P)

Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), Granada, Spain.
University of Granada, Spain.

Articles similaires

[Redispensing of expensive oral anticancer medicines: a practical application].

Lisanne N van Merendonk, Kübra Akgöl, Bastiaan Nuijen
1.00
Humans Antineoplastic Agents Administration, Oral Drug Costs Counterfeit Drugs

Smoking Cessation and Incident Cardiovascular Disease.

Jun Hwan Cho, Seung Yong Shin, Hoseob Kim et al.
1.00
Humans Male Smoking Cessation Cardiovascular Diseases Female
Humans United States Aged Cross-Sectional Studies Medicare Part C
1.00
Humans Yoga Low Back Pain Female Male

Classifications MeSH